D. A. Allport, Selection for action: Some behaviorial and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action, Perspectives on perception and action, pp.395-419, 1987.

N. J. Hillsdale,

F. Arcediano, M. Escobar, and R. R. Miller, Bidirectional associations in humans and rats, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, vol.31, pp.301-318, 2005.

T. Astor-jack and P. Haggard, Intention and reactivity, Attention in action: Advances from cognitive neuroscience, pp.109-130, 2005.

T. Beckers, J. De-houwer, and P. Eelen, Automatic integration of non-perceptual action effect features: The case of the associative affective Simon effect, Psychological Research, vol.66, pp.166-173, 2002.

J. Cohen, Statistical power analysis for the social sciences, 1988.

M. P. Deiber, V. Ibanez, N. Sadato, and M. Hallett, Cerebral structures participating in motor preparation in humans: A positron emission tomography study, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.75, pp.233-247, 1996.

F. C. Donders, On the speed of mental processes, Koster, Trans.). Acta Psychologica, vol.30, pp.412-431, 1868.

B. Elsner and B. Hommel, Effect anticipation and action control, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, vol.27, pp.229-240, 2001.

B. Elsner and B. Hommel, Contiguity and contingency in the acquisition of action effects, Psychological Research, vol.68, pp.138-154, 2004.

B. Elsner, B. Hommel, C. Mentschel, A. Drzezga, W. Prinz et al., Linking actions and their perceivable consequences in the human brain, NeuroImage, vol.17, pp.364-372, 2002.

S. Exner, Physiologie der Grosshirnrinde, 1879.

, Handbuch der Physiologie, vol.2, pp.189-350

F. Faul and E. Erdfelder, GPOWER: A priori, post hoc, and compromise power analyses for MS-DOS, 1992.

G. Bonn,

E. Fox, Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, vol.2, pp.145-173, 1995.

P. Haggard, G. Aschersleben, J. Gehrke, and W. Prinz, Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance XIX, pp.266-285, 2002.

P. Haggard, S. Clark, and J. Kalogeras, Voluntary action and conscious awareness, Nature Neuroscience, vol.5, pp.382-385, 2002.

E. Harless, Der Apparat des Willens, Zeitschrift für Philosophie und Philosophische Kritik, vol.38, pp.50-73, 1861.

J. Hoffmann, A. Sebald, and C. Stoecker, Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, vol.27, pp.470-482, 2001.

B. Hommel, The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects, Psychological Research, vol.59, pp.176-186, 1996.

B. Hommel, Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus -response episodes, Visual Cognition, vol.5, pp.183-216, 1998.

B. Hommel, The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus -response translation, Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII, pp.247-273, 2000.

B. Hommel, Coloring an action: Intending to produce color events eliminates the Stroop effect, Psychological Research, vol.68, pp.74-90, 2004.

B. Hommel, B. Pösse, and F. Waszak, Contextualization in perception and action, Psychologica Belgica, vol.40, pp.227-245, 2000.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02385737

P. Keller, E. Wascher, W. Prinz, F. Waszak, I. Koch et al., Differences between intention-based and stimulus-based actions, Journal of Psychophysiology, vol.20, pp.9-20, 2006.

W. Kunde, Response priming by supraliminal and subliminal action effects, Psychological Research, vol.68, pp.91-96, 2004.

W. Kunde, J. Hoffmann, and P. Zellmann, The impact of anticipated action effects on action planning, Acta Psychologica, vol.109, pp.137-155, 2002.

F. Lhermitte, Utilisation behaviour" and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes, Brain, vol.106, pp.237-255, 1983.

B. Libet, C. A. Gleason, E. W. Wright, and D. K. Pearl, Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act, Brain, vol.106, pp.623-642, 1983.

G. D. Logan, Toward an instance theory of automatization, Psychological Review, vol.95, pp.492-527, 1988.

G. D. Logan and J. L. Etherton, What is learned during automatization? The role of attention in constructing an instance, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, vol.20, pp.1022-1050, 1994.

R. H. Lotze, Medizinische Psychologie oder die Physiologie der Seele, 1852.

G. Leipzig, Weidmann'sche Buchhandlung

W. T. Neill, Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, vol.23, pp.1291-1305, 1997.

W. T. Neill and L. A. Valdes, Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, vol.18, pp.565-576, 1992.

D. Norman and T. Shallice, Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior, Consciousness and self regulation: Advances in research and theory, vol.4, pp.1-18, 1986.

R. E. Passingham, I. Toni, and M. F. Rushworth, Specialisation within the prefrontal cortex: The ventral prefrontal cortex and associative learning, Experimental Brain Research, vol.133, pp.103-113, 2000.

W. Prinz, Perception and action planning, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, vol.9, pp.129-154, 1997.

T. Shallice, P. W. Burgess, F. Schon, and D. M. Baxton, The origins of utilization behavior, Brain, vol.112, pp.1587-1598, 1989.

G. Thut, C. A. Hauert, P. Viviani, S. Morand, L. Spinelli et al., Internally driven vs. externally cued movement selection: A study on the timing of brain activity, Cognitive Brain Research, vol.9, pp.261-269, 2000.

I. Toni, M. F. Rushworth, and R. E. Passingham, Neural correlates of visuomotor associations: Spatial rules compared with arbitrary rules, Experimental Brain Research, vol.141, pp.359-369, 2001.

F. Waszak and B. Hommel, The costs and benefits of cross-task priming. Memory and Cognition

F. Waszak, B. Hommel, and A. Allport, Taskswitching and long-term priming: Role of episodic S-R bindings in task-shift costs, Cognitive Psychology, vol.46, pp.361-413, 2003.

F. Waszak, B. Hommel, and A. Allport, Interaction of task readiness and automatic retrieval in task switching: Negative priming and competitor priming, Memory & Cognition, vol.33, pp.595-610, 2005.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02385884

F. Waszak, E. Wascher, P. Keller, I. Koch, G. Aschersleben et al., Intention-based and stimulus-based mechanisms in action selection, Experimental Brain Research, vol.162, pp.346-356, 2005.

M. Ziessler, Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, vol.24, pp.962-978, 1998.

M. Ziessler and D. Nattkemper, Learning of event sequences is based on response effect learning: Further evidence from a serial reaction task, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, vol.27, pp.595-613, 2001.

M. Ziessler and D. Nattkemper, Effect anticipation in action planning: Anticipative learning of action effects, Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance XIX, pp.645-672, 2002.

U. K. Oxford,