D. B. Allen, R. , and K. P. , Perception of simple figures drawn upon the body surface, Percept. Mot. Skills, vol.30, pp.369-370, 1970.

A. J. Alsmith, E. R. Ferrè, and M. R. Longo, Dissociating contributions of head and torso to spatial reference frames: the misalignment paradigm, Conscious. Cogn, vol.53, pp.105-114, 2017.

A. J. Alsmith and M. R. Longo, Where exactly am I? Self-location judgements distribute between head and torso, Conscious. Cogn, vol.24, pp.70-74, 2014.

M. A. Amorim, B. Isableu, and M. Jarraya, Body analogy" for the mental rotation of objects, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen, vol.135, pp.327-347, 2006.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00579448

G. Arnold, A. , and M. , The graphesthesia paradigm: drawing letters on the body to investigate the embodied nature of spatial perspective taking. I-Perception, vol.8, pp.1-5, 2017.

G. Arnold, C. Spence, A. , and M. , Taking someone else's spatial perspective: natural stance or effortful decentring?, Cognition, vol.148, pp.27-33, 2016.

G. Arnold, C. Spence, A. , and M. , A unity of the self or a multiplicity of locations? How the graphesthesia task sheds light on the role of spatial perspectives in bodily self-consciousness, Conscious. Cogn, vol.56, pp.100-114, 2017.

M. Barnett-cowan, R. T. Dyde, C. Thompson, and L. R. Harris, , 2010.

M. Barnett-cowan and L. R. Harris, Perceived self-orientation in allocentric and egocentric space: effects of visual and physical tilt on saccadic and tactile measures, Eur. J. Neurosci, vol.31, pp.231-243, 2008.

F. Bertossa, M. Besa, R. Ferrari, and F. Ferri, Point zero: a phenomenological inquiry into the seat of consciousness, Percept. Mot. Skills, vol.107, pp.323-335, 2008.

O. Blanke, Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness, Nat. Rev. Neurosci, vol.13, pp.556-571, 2012.

J. Blouin, C. Bard, N. Teasdale, J. Paillard, M. Fleury et al., Reference systems for coding spatial information in normal subjects and a deafferented patient, Exp. Brain Res, vol.93, pp.324-331, 1993.

J. Blouin, N. Teasdale, and L. Mouchnino, Vestibular signal processing in a subject with somatosensory deafferentation: the case of sitting posture, BMC Neurol, vol.7, p.25, 2007.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00321005

L. Bringoux, C. Scotto-di-cesare, L. Borel, T. Macaluso, and F. R. Sarlegna, Do visual and vestibular inputs compensate for somatosensory loss in the perception of spatial orientation? Insights from a deafferented patient, Front. Hum. Neurosci, vol.10, p.181, 2016.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01412210

A. Cohen and I. Lewin, Internal versus external references with respect to perception of stimuli on the body surface, Percept. Mot. Skills, vol.63, pp.1039-1046, 1986.

Y. E. Cohen and R. A. Andersen, A common reference frame for movement plans in the posterior parietal cortex, Nat. Rev. Neurosci, vol.3, pp.553-562, 2002.

J. Cole and J. Paillard, Living without touch and peripheral information about body position and movement: studies with deafferented subjects" in The body and the self, pp.245-266, 1995.

D. Deroualle, M. Toupet, C. Van-nechel, U. Duquesne, C. Hautefort et al., Anchoring the self to the body in bilateral vestibular failure, PLoS One, vol.12, 2017.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02121685

D. Vignemont and F. , Mind the body: An exploration of bodily self-awareness, 2018.

J. D. Duke, Perception of finger drawings upon the body surface, J. Gen. Psychol, vol.75, pp.305-314, 1966.

R. T. Dyde, M. R. Jenkin, and L. R. Harris, The subjective visual vertical and the perceptual upright, Exp. Brain Res, vol.173, pp.612-622, 2006.

E. R. Ferrè, C. Lopez, and P. Haggard, Anchoring the self to the body: vestibular contribution to the sense of self, Psychol. Sci, vol.25, pp.2106-2108, 2014.

S. Gallagher, C. , and J. , Body image and body schema in a deafferented subject, The Journal of Mind and Behavior, vol.16, pp.369-389, 1995.

V. Harrar and L. R. Harris, Touch used to guide action is partially coded in a visual reference frame, Exp. Brain Res, vol.203, pp.615-620, 2010.

L. R. Harris, M. J. Carnevale, S. D'-amour, L. E. Fraser, V. Harrar et al., How our body influences our perception of the world, Front. Psychol, vol.6, p.819, 2015.

C. Ho and C. Spence, Head orientation biases tactile localization, Brain Res, vol.1144, pp.136-141, 2007.

K. Kessler and L. A. Thomson, The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference, Cognition, vol.114, pp.72-88, 2010.

D. Krech and R. S. Crutchfield, Elements of psychology, 1958.

J. R. Lackner, Some proprioceptive influences on the perceptual representation of body shape and orientation, Brain, vol.111, pp.281-297, 1988.

J. Limanowski and H. Hecht, Where do we stand on locating the self?, Psychology, vol.2, pp.312-317, 2011.

R. C. Miall, N. M. Kitchen, S. Nam, H. Lefumat, A. G. Renault et al., Proprioceptive loss and the perception, control and learning of arm movements in humans: evidence from sensory neuronopathy, Exp. Brain Res, vol.236, pp.2137-2155, 2018.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01917544

T. Natsoulas and R. A. Dubanovski, Inferring the locus and orientation of the perceiver from responses to stimulation of the skin, Am. J. Psychol, vol.77, pp.281-285, 1964.

V. Nougier, C. Bard, M. Fleury, N. Teasdale, J. Cole et al., Control of single-joint movements in deafferented patients: evidence for amplitude coding rather than position control, Exp. Brain Res, vol.109, pp.473-482, 1996.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00193927

J. O'brien, A. , and M. , Cognition overrides orientation dependence in tactile viewpoint selection, Exp. Brain Res, vol.234, pp.1885-1892, 2016.

P. K. Oltman, A portable rod-and-frame apparatus, Percept. Mot. Skills, vol.26, pp.503-506, 1968.

L. M. Parsons and S. Shimojo, Perceived spatial organization of cutaneous patterns on surfaces of the human body in various positions, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform, vol.13, pp.488-504, 1987.

L. M. Pritchett, M. J. Carnevale, and L. R. Harris, Reference frames for coding touch location depend on the task, Exp. Brain Res, vol.222, pp.437-445, 2012.

F. Quesque, E. Chabanat, and Y. Rossetti, Taking the point of view of the blind: spontaneous level-2 perspective-taking in irrelevant conditions, J. Exp. Soc. Psychol, vol.79, pp.356-364, 2018.

A. G. Renault, M. Auvray, G. Parseihian, R. C. Miall, J. Cole et al., Does proprioception influence human spatial cognition? A study on individuals with massive deafferentation, Front. Psychol, vol.9, p.1322, 2018.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01917539

R. L. Sainburg, H. Poizner, and C. Ghez, Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination, J. Neurophysiol, vol.70, pp.2136-2147, 1993.

F. R. Sarlegna, N. Malfait, L. Bringoux, C. Bourdin, and J. L. Vercher, Force-field adaptation without proprioception: can vision be used to model limb dynamics?, Neuropsychologia, vol.48, pp.60-67, 2010.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01384815

K. Sekiyama, Importance of head axes in perception of cutaneous patterns drawn on vertical body surfaces, Percept. Psychophys, vol.49, pp.481-492, 1991.

T. Horst, A. C. Cole, J. Van-lier, R. Steenbergen, and B. , The effect of chronic deafferentation on mental imagery: a case study, PLoS One, vol.7, 2012.

B. Tversky and B. M. Hard, Embodied and disembodied cognition: spatial perspective-taking, Cognition, vol.110, pp.124-129, 2009.