J. Bastin, S. Calvin, and G. Montagne, Muscular proprioception contributes to the control of interceptive actions, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and Performance, vol.32, p.964972, 2006.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/inserm-00410946

J. Bastin, C. Craig, and G. Montagne, Prospective strategies underlie the control of interceptive actions, Human Movement Science, vol.25, p.71832, 2006.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/inserm-00410949

J. Bastin, D. M. Jacobs, A. H. Morice, C. Craig, and G. Montagne, Testing the role of expansion in the prospective control of locomotion, Experimental Brain Research, vol.191, p.301312, 2008.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/inserm-00410933

J. K. Caird and P. A. Hancock, The perception of arrival time for dierent oncoming vehicles at an intersection, Ecological Psychology, vol.6, p.83109, 1994.

A. Chardenon, G. Montagne, M. J. Buekers, and M. Laurent, The visual control of ball interception during human locomotion, Neuroscience letters, vol.334, p.1316, 2002.

A. Chardenon, G. Montagne, M. Laurent, and R. J. Bootsma, The perceptual control of goal-directed locomotion: a common control architecture for interception and navigation? Experimental Brain Research 158, 2004.

A. Chardenon, G. Montagne, M. Laurent, and R. J. Bootsma, A robust solution for dealing with environmental changes in intercepting moving balls, Journal of Motor Behavior, vol.37, p.5262, 2005.

B. J. Chihak, J. M. Plumert, C. J. Ziemer, S. Babu, T. Grechkin et al., Synchronizing self and object movement: How and adult cyclists intercept moving gaps in a virtual environment, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.36, p.15351552, 2010.

H. E. Clark, J. A. Perrone, and R. B. Isler, An illusory sizespeed bias and railway crossing collisions, Accident Analysis & Prevention, vol.55, p.226231, 2013.

H. E. Clark, J. A. Perrone, R. B. Isler, and S. G. Charlton, The role of eye movements in the size-speed illusion of approaching trains, Accident Analysis & Prevention, vol.86, 2016.

G. J. Diaz, F. Phillips, and B. R. Fajen, Intercepting moving targets: a little foresight helps a lot, p.345360, 2009.

S. Espie and J. M. Auberlet, ARCHISIM: a behavioral multi-actors trac simulation model for the study of a trac system including ITS aspects, International Journal of ITS Research, vol.5, p.716, 2007.

M. François, A. Morice, J. Blouin, and G. Montagne, Age-related decline in sensory processing for locomotion and interception, Neuroscience, vol.172, p.366378, 2011.

K. Ghose, T. K. Horiuchi, P. S. Krishnaprasad, and C. F. Moss, Echolocating bats use a nearly Time-Optimal strategy to intercept prey, PLoS Biol, vol.4, 2006.

M. Gould, D. R. Poulter, S. Helman, and J. P. Wann, Judgments of approach speed for motorcycles across dierent lighting levels and the eect of an improved tri-headlight conguration, Accid. Anal. Prev URL, 2012.

P. Hancock and M. Manster, Time-to-contact: More than tau alone, Ecological Psychology, vol.9, 1997.

P. A. Hancock, J. K. Caird, S. Shekhar, and M. Vercruyssen, Factors Inuencing Drivers Left Turn Decisions, Annual Meeting Proceedings, 1991.

M. S. Horswill, S. Helman, P. Ardiles, and J. P. Wann, Motorcycle accident risk could be inated by a time to arrival illusion, Optometry & Vision Science, vol.82, p.740746, 2005.

B. Lanchester and R. Mark, Pursuit and prediction in the tracking of moving food by a teleost sh (acanthaluteres spilomelanurus), The Journal of Experimental Biology, vol.63, p.627645, 1975.

D. Le-brun, J. Bordier, and F. Le-guern, Nouveau manuel du marin. Solar, 2007.

D. N. Lee, A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision, Perception, vol.5, p.437459, 1976.

M. Lenoir, E. Musch, E. Thiery, and G. J. Savelsbergh, Rate of change of angular bearing as the relevant property in a horizontal interception task during locomotion, Journal of Motor Behavior, vol.34, p.385401, 2002.

M. Lenoir, G. J. Savelsbergh, E. Musch, E. Thiery, J. Uyttenhove et al., Intercepting moving objects during self-motion: effects of environmental changes, Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, vol.70, p.34960, 1999.

N. Louveton, R. Bootsma, P. Guerrin, C. Berthelon, and G. Montagne, Intersection crossing considered as intercepting a moving trac gap: effects of task and environmental constraints, Acta Psychologica, vol.141, p.294, 2012.

N. Louveton, G. Montagne, C. Berthelon, and R. Bootsma, Crossing through a dynamical gap: which perceptual features of the gap matter?, Human Movement Science, vol.31, p.15001516, 2012.

R. Olberg, Visual control of prey-capture ight in dragonies, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, vol.22, 2011.

R. Olberg, A. Worthington, and K. Venator, Prey pursuit and interception in dragonies, Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, vol.186, p.155162, 2000.

T. Petzoldt, Size speed bias or size arrival eecthow judgments of vehicles' approach speed and time to arrival are inuenced by the vehicles' size, Accident Analysis & Prevention, vol.95, p.132137, 2016.

D. Regan and S. J. Hamstra, Dissociation of discrimination thresholds for time to contact and for rate of angular expansion, Vision Research, vol.33, issue.93, p.90252, 1993.

S. Rossel, J. Corlija, and S. Schuster, Predicting three-dimensional target motion: How archer sh determine where to catch their dislodged prey, Journal of Experimental Biology, vol.205, p.33213326, 2002.

A. De-rugy, G. Montagne, M. J. Buekers, and M. Laurent, Spatially constrained locomotion under informational conict, Behavioural Brain Research, vol.123, p.1115, 2001.