G. Abbruzzese, S. Ratto, M. Favale, and M. Abbruzzese, Proprioceptive modulation of somatosensory evoked potentials during active or passive finger movements in man., Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, vol.44, issue.10, pp.942-949, 1981.
DOI : 10.1136/jnnp.44.10.942

S. Allain, B. Burle, T. Hasbroucq, and F. Vidal, Sequential adjustments before and after partial errors, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, vol.111, issue.2, pp.356-362, 2009.
DOI : 10.3758/PBR.16.2.356

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00383801

S. Allain, L. Carbonnell, B. Burle, T. Hasbroucq, and F. Vidal, On-line executive control: An electromyographic study, Psychophysiology, vol.1, issue.1, pp.113-116, 2004.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2003.00136.x

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01384902

S. Allain, T. Hasbroucq, B. Burle, J. Grapperon, and F. Vidal, Response monitoring without sensory feedback, Clinical Neurophysiology, vol.115, issue.9, pp.2014-2020, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.04.013

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01384912

R. W. Angel and R. C. Malenka, Velocity-dependent suppression of cutaneous sensitivity during movement, Experimental Neurology, vol.77, issue.2, pp.266-274, 1972.
DOI : 10.1016/0014-4886(82)90244-8

B. Burle and M. Bonnet, What???s an internal clock for?, Behavioural Processes, vol.45, issue.1-3, pp.59-72, 1999.
DOI : 10.1016/S0376-6357(99)00009-1

B. Burle, C. Possamaï, F. Vidal, M. Bonnet, and T. Hasbroucq, Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis, Psychological Research, vol.66, issue.4, pp.324-336, 2002.
DOI : 10.1007/s00426-002-0105-6

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01384935

B. Burle, C. Roger, S. Allain, F. Vidal, and T. Hasbroucq, Error Negativity Does Not Reflect Conflict: A Reappraisal of Conflict Monitoring and Anterior Cingulate Cortex Activity, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol.22, issue.9, pp.1637-1655, 2008.
DOI : 10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.931

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00320988

C. S. Carter, T. S. Braver, D. M. Barch, M. M. Botvinick, D. Noll et al., Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Error Detection, and the Online Monitoring of Performance, Science, vol.280, issue.5364, pp.747-749, 1998.
DOI : 10.1126/science.280.5364.747

M. G. Coles, M. K. Scheffers, and C. B. Holroyd, Why is there an ERN/Ne on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error-processing, Biological Psychology, vol.56, issue.3, pp.173-189, 2001.
DOI : 10.1016/S0301-0511(01)00076-X

S. Dehaene and L. Naccache, Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework, Cognition, vol.79, issue.1-2, pp.1-37, 2001.
DOI : 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2

E. Delong, D. Delong, and D. Clarke-pearson, Comparing the Areas under Two or More Correlated Receiver Operating Characteristic Curves: A Nonparametric Approach, Biometrics, vol.44, issue.3, pp.837-845, 1988.
DOI : 10.2307/2531595

G. Dutilh, J. Vandekerckhove, B. U. Forstmann, E. Keuleers, M. Brysbaert et al., Testing theories of post-error slowing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, pp.454-465, 2012.

T. Endrass, B. Reuter, and N. Kathmann, ERP correlates of conscious error recognition: aware and unaware errors in an antisaccade task, European Journal of Neuroscience, vol.51, issue.6, pp.1714-1720, 2007.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05785.x

E. V. Evarts, Relation of pyramidal tract activity to force exerted during voluntary movement, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.31, pp.14-27, 1968.

M. Falkenstein, J. Hohnsbein, J. Hoormann, and L. Blanke, Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, vol.78, issue.6, pp.447-455, 1991.
DOI : 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90062-9

W. J. Gehring, B. Goss, M. G. Coles, D. E. Meyer, and E. Donchin, A NEURAL SYSTEM FOR ERROR DETECTION AND COMPENSATION, Psychological Science, vol.9, issue.6, pp.385-390, 1993.
DOI : 10.1016/0006-8993(89)91222-5

G. Gescheider, Psychophysics, theory and method, 1976.

G. Gratton, M. G. Coles, and E. Donchin, Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation of responses., Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, vol.121, issue.4, pp.480-506, 1992.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.480

D. Green and J. A. Swets, Signal detection theory and psychophysics, 1966.

P. Haggard and E. Magno, Localising awareness of action with transcranial magnetic stimulation, Experimental Brain Research, vol.127, issue.1, pp.102-107, 1999.
DOI : 10.1007/s002210050778

P. Haggard and B. Whitford, Supplementary motor area provides an efferent signal for sensory suppression, Cognitive Brain Research, vol.19, issue.1, pp.52-58, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.018

T. Hasbroucq, C. Possamaï, M. Bonnet, F. Vidal, M. H. Kutner et al., Effect of the irrelevant location of the response signal on choice reaction time: An electromyographic study in humans, Psychophysiology, vol.36, issue.4, pp.522-526, 1999.
DOI : 10.1017/S0048577299001602

D. R. Laming, Information theory of choice-reaction times, 1968.

D. Laming, Choice reaction performance following an error, Acta Psychologica, vol.43, issue.3, pp.199-224, 1979.
DOI : 10.1016/0001-6918(79)90026-X

H. Masaki and S. Segalowitz, Error negativity: A test of the response conflict versus error detection hypotheses, 2004.

P. R. Murphy, I. H. Robertson, D. Allen, R. Hester, and R. G. Connell, An electrophysiological signal that precisely tracks the emergence of error awareness, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol.6, 2012.
DOI : 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00065

S. Nieuwenhuis, K. R. Ridderinkhof, J. Blom, G. P. Band, and A. Kok, Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task, Psychophysiology, vol.38, issue.5, pp.752-760, 2001.
DOI : 10.1111/1469-8986.3850752

W. Notebaert, F. Houtman, F. Van-opstal, W. Gevers, W. Fias et al., Post-error slowing: An orienting account, Cognition, vol.111, issue.2, pp.275-279, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.002

O. Connell, R. G. Dockree, P. M. Bellgrove, M. A. Kelly, S. P. Hester et al., The role of cingulate cortex in the detection of errors with and without awareness: a high-density electrical mapping study, European Journal of Neuroscience, vol.112, issue.8, pp.2571-2579, 2007.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05477.x

R. Development and C. Team, R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from www.R-project.org, 2012.

P. M. Rabbitt, Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.71, issue.2, pp.264-272, 1966.
DOI : 10.1037/h0022853

P. M. Rabbitt, Consciousness is slower than you think, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, vol.33, issue.4, pp.1081-1092, 2002.
DOI : 10.1016/0001-6918(67)90054-6

P. M. Rabbitt and S. M. Vyas, Processing a display even after you make a response to it. How perceptual errors can be corrected, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, vol.27, issue.11, pp.223-239, 1981.
DOI : 10.1037/h0033872

R. Ratcliff and G. Mckoon, The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks, Neural Computation, vol.86, issue.4, pp.12-18, 2008.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3733-05.2006

X. Robin, N. Turck, A. Hainard, N. Tiberti, F. Lisacek et al., pROC: an open-source package for R and S+ to analyze and compare ROC curves, BMC Bioinformatics, vol.12, issue.1, p.77, 2011.
DOI : 10.1007/s00134-009-1641-y

C. Roger, C. G. Bénar, F. Vidal, T. Hasbroucq, and B. Burle, Rostral Cingulate Zone and correct response monitoring: ICA and source localization evidences for the unicity of correct- and error-negativities, NeuroImage, vol.51, issue.1, pp.391-403, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.005

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01293542

M. K. Scheffers, M. G. Coles, P. Bernstein, W. J. Gehring, and E. Donchin, Event-related brain potentials and error-related processing: An analysis of incorrect responses to go and no-go stimuli, Psychophysiology, vol.21, issue.1, pp.42-53, 1996.
DOI : 10.1016/0001-6918(67)90053-4

J. R. Simon, The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing, Stimulus?response compatibility: An integrated perspective, pp.31-86, 1990.
DOI : 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61218-2

H. G. Smid, G. Mulder, L. J. Mulder, and N. Yeung, Selective response activation can begin before stimulus recognition is complete: A psychophysiological and error analysis of continuous flow, Acta Psychologica, vol.74, issue.2-3, pp.169-210, 1990.
DOI : 10.1016/0001-6918(90)90005-Z

M. Stevens, J. Lammertyn, F. Verbruggen, A. Vandierendonck, G. J. Van-boxtel et al., Tscope: A C library for programming cognitive experiments on the MS Windows platform, Behavior Research Methods, vol.34, issue.2, pp.280-286, 1993.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03192779

S. Van-gaal, V. A. Lamme, and K. R. Ridderinkhof, Unconsciously Triggered Conflict Adaptation, PLoS ONE, vol.7, issue.7, 2010.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0011508.t001

F. Vidal, T. Hasbroucq, J. Grapperon, and M. Bonnet, Is the ???error negativity??? specific to errors?, Biological Psychology, vol.51, issue.2-3, pp.109-128, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00032-0

S. I. Vrieze, Model selection and psychological theory: A discussion of the differences between the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) Computational approaches to motor control, Psychological Methods Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.17, issue.1, pp.228-243, 1997.